indexed_doc en utf8 Kristine Turner Information Seeking, Retrieving, Reading, and Storing Behaviour of Library Users. http://Scratch/ak19/gs2-diffcol-26Mar2018/collect/DSpace-To-GS/tmp/1522032971_2/5.html http://Scratch/ak19/gs2-diffcol-26Mar2018/collect/DSpace-To-GS/tmp/1522032971_2/5.html import/5/5.pdf tmp/1522032971_2/5.html 5.html 5.pdf 5.pdf PDFPlugin 220303 5 PDF _iconpdf_ doc.pdf doc.pdf 28 8.57 /Scratch/ak19/gs2-diffcol-26Mar2018/collect/DSpace-To-GS/import/5 2018:03:26 15:55:32+13:00 5.pdf 775 220303 PDF application/pdf mdewsnip 2006:05:30 14:50:53+12:00 PScript5.dll Version 5.2.2 false 2006:05:30 14:50:53+12:00 1.4 28 Acrobat Distiller 7.0.5 &#40;Windows&#41; Information Seeking&#44; Retrieving&#44; Reading&#44; and Storing Behaviour of LIbrary-Users. 2006:05:30 14:50:53+12:00 PScript5.dll Version 5.2.2 PScript5.dll Version 5.2.2 uuid:5bfa2f58-a06e-4ba1-97a7-89f2153cd2af application/pdf uuid:9179f25d-0de5-4e72-a113-e79857787e94 2006:05:30 14:58:55+12:00 2006:05:30 14:50:53+12:00 Acrobat Distiller 7.0.5 &#40;Windows&#41; Information Seeking&#44; Retrieving&#44; Reading&#44; and Storing Behaviour of LIbrary-Users. 3.1-702 Kristine Turner 2005-01-10T02:46:14Z 2005-01-10T02:46:14Z 2005-01-10T02:46:14Z en Information Seeking, Retrieving, Reading, and Storing Behaviour of Library Users <a href="_httpprefix_/collect/[collection]/index/assoc/{Or}{[parent(Top):assocfilepath],[assocfilepath]}/5.doc">{If}{_icondoc_,_icondoc_,doc}</a> <a href='_httpprefix_/collect/[collection]/index/assoc/[assocfilepath]/5.doc'> _icondoc_ </a> <a href="_httpprefix_/collect/[collection]/index/assoc/{Or}{[parent(Top):assocfilepath],[assocfilepath]}/5.doc">{If}{_icondoc_,_icondoc_,doc}</a> HASHd6b757362e763ba6d6b757 1522032932 20180326 1522032972 20180326 HASHd6b7.dir doc.pdf:application/pdf: 5.doc:application/msword: <A name=1></a><b>Information Seeking, Retrieving, Reading, and Storing Behaviour of </b><br> <b>Library-Users. </b><br> <br> <br> Kristine Turner <br> email: kristine@clear.net.nz <br> <br> <br><b>Abstract <br></b> <br>In the interest of digital libraries, it is advisable that designers be aware of the <br>potential behaviour of the users of such a system. There are two distinct parts under <br>investigation, the interaction between traditional libraries involving the seeking and <br>retrieval of relevant material, and the reading and storage behaviours ensuing. <br>Through this analysis, the findings could be incorporated into digital library facilities. <br>There has been copious amounts of research on information seeking leading to the <br>development of behavioural models to describe the process. Often research on the <br>information seeking practices of individuals is based on the task and field of study. <br>The information seeking model, presented by Ellis <i>et al.</i> (1993), characterises the <br>format of this study where it is used to compare various research on the information <br>seeking practices of groups of people (from academics to professionals). It is found <br>that, although researchers do make use of library facilities, they tend to rely heavily <br>on their own collections and primarily use the library as a source for previously <br>identified information, browsing and interloan. It was found that there are significant <br>differences in user behaviour between the groups analysed. When looking at the <br>reading and storage of material it was hard to draw conclusions, due to the lack of <br>substantial research and information on the topic. However, through the use of <br>reading strategies, a general idea on how readers behave can be developed. Designers <br>of digital libraries can benefit from the guidelines presented here to better understand <br>their audience. <br> <br><b>Introduction <br></b> <br>“The migration of information from paper to electronic media promises to change the <br>whole nature of research” (Witten <i>et al.</i> 1995). Through the advent of office <br>computers and the transformation of media, the popularity and usage of digital <br>libraries has increased. Researchers can benefit from the search, retrieval, reading <br>and storage facilities available to them from the comfort and convenience of their own <br>chair. An important issue in this day of human-computer interaction is that not only <br>the information needs of these researchers are meet, but user requirements also. <br> <br>To cater for researchers, it is in the interest of digital library designers to investigate <br>and understand user behaviour. Ignorance in understanding how human behaviours <br>influence digital libraries can lead to a potential risk of design inadequacies. A <br>consequence is that digital libraries may not satisfy the requirements of users. To <br>rectify this problem, an investigation and summary of the main research surrounding <br>user behaviour of traditional libraries is presented here. By studying the user <br>behaviour in traditional libraries and how they seek, retrieve, read and store selected <br>materials, one can begin to understand how these attributes can be used to enhance the <br>search and delivery facilities of a digital library. <br> <hr> <A name=2></a> <br>There are specifically two components that are addressed which are distinct in nature <br>and shed light on the behaviour of library users: library-user interaction, and <br>information use and storage. Library-user behaviour covers the information seeking <br>process — from acknowledging a need of specific information to the delivery of the <br>relevant material required to resolve the need. This paper looks at this process and the <br>activities involved in relation to traditional libraries. When looking into the usage and <br>storage of information, the reading behaviours involved in extracting information <br>from retrieved material was investigated. This focussed primarily on conventional <br>reading environments and methods, and document presentation and storage. The goal <br>is to begin to understand how researchers find and use information based on the <br>findings of previous studies. <br> <br><b>Library-User Interaction <br></b> <br><i>Information Seeking and Retrieval <br></i>Different search techniques are undertaken by library users to search and locate <br>relevant information. To understand how users of libraries search and locate relevant <br>documents we need to understand the search techniques and what resources and <br>sources of information they generally use. <br> <br>There are many ways of looking at the information seeking process. Of the research <br>viewed, each one had its own ideals and factors that shed new light on the activities <br>conducted. Ford (1973) offers a conceptual model for researching information needs <br>and uses on the basis of information communication. The model has six components <br>— sources or originators, methods or activities, messages, channels or media, <br>recipients, and information. It is presented as: <br> <br> (SOURCE) (METHOD) <br> (MESSAGES) <br> “The source / writes or speaks / ideas, research results, etc. / which are trans- <br> (CHANNEL) (RECIPIENT) (METHOD) <br>mitted by / journal, meeting, etc, etc. / to the recipient, who reads or hears / <br>the message and is thus informed. At this point the message is converted <br>into INFORMATION” (Ford 1973, p. 85). <br> <br>This view of information flow can aid in researching information seeking and <br>retrieval practices by providing a basis to analyse interactions. <br> <br>In contrast, Kuhlthau (1993) offers an uncertainty principle as a framework for <br>understanding how individuals conduct information seeking. The article looks at the <br>feelings, thoughts and actions associated with information seeking as a person <br>“move[s] from ambiguity to specificity, or ... uncertainty to understanding” (Kuhlthau <br>1993, p. 340), and argues that information seeking cannot be based on certainty and <br>order as these are variables which fluctuate and need to be considered. The <br>information seeking tasks identified by Kuhlthau (1993) are: initiation, an awareness <br>of an information need; selection, the identification or selection of an approach or <br>subject to explore; exploration, the investigation of information to gain understanding; <br>formulation, where the person gains a perspective or point of view on the problem; <br>collection, the gathering of the relevant information; and presentation, to fulfil the <br> <hr> <A name=3></a>information need and conclude the search. Through these stages of information <br>seeking, the individual is subject to feelings of uncertainty, optimism, confusion, <br>frustration, doubt, clarity, sense of direction, confidence, and satisfaction or <br>disappointment. Actions move from exploration to the documentation stage; thoughts <br>move from being vague in the earlier stages to being focussed as interest increases <br>(Kuhlthau 1993, p. 343, figure 1). <br> <br>Often research on information seeking practices is characterised by an individual’s <br>task or problem (Mick <i>et al.</i> 1980; Belkin <i>et al.</i> 1982; Ingwersen 1992 found in <br>Bystrom and Jarvelin 1995). These studies investigate the relationship between a <br>person’s task (for example, in sciences, social sciences, humanities) and their <br>information seeking behaviour. Bystrom and Jarvelin (1995) acknowledges that <br>people’s information seeking depends on their task and it looks at how task <br>complexity can be used to model information needs, seeking, channels and sources. <br>However, other research shows that task alone may not be specific enough to analyse <br>the behaviour of information seekers and users. They argue that other factors other <br>then tasks may contribute to information seeking behaviours (Kuhlthau 1993). <br> <br>Those papers that characterise information seeking practices based on tasks, surveyed <br>scholars and professionals in particular fields to determine similarities and <br>generalisations within and between these groups of people. These determine an <br>overall way in which certain groups of people search for information and their needs <br>and uses of it. Studies reviewed looked at the scientific community (Ellis <i>et al.</i> 1993; <br>Hallmark 1994; Seggern 1995), computer sciences (Cunningham and Connaway), <br>social sciences (Folster 1995), humanities (Broadbent 1986; Wiberley and Jones <br>1989), and professionals (Leckie <i>et al.</i> 1996). There are other more specific studies, <br>such as anthropology (Hartmann 1995), philosophy (Sievert and Sievert 1989), and <br>engineering (Pinelli 1991; Holland and Powell 1995). This classification of people <br>means that in general it is easy to determine the type of behaviour expected from an <br>individual based on their task or field of interest. <br> <br>This paper makes utilises these communities of people to describe information <br>seeking and retrieval activities. However, it has to be noted that, although <br>categorising provides good generalisations of information seeking behaviour there are <br>often conflicts. This is demonstrated in the study by Pinelli (1991), where the <br>information seeking practices of scientists and engineers are compared. In the past <br>these two groups of people have been studied synonymously. It has now been <br>determined that the differences in their behaviour is quite distinct. For instance, <br>engineers make more use of unpublished technical material than their academic <br>counterparts. This shows that even with similar or related communities, there may be <br>considerable differences in information seeking behaviour (Pinelli 1991). <br> <br>Research generally agrees on how people go about searching for information. Ellis <i>et <br>al.</i> (1993) discusses interviews conducted on information and diffusion activities, <br>focussing specifically on the information seeking habits of physicists and chemists. It <br>offers characteristics of the information gathering activities for these scientists, in <br>comparison to social scientists, and presents a behaviour model. While these <br>activities are associated with a particular group of people, they can be generalised to <br>encompass scholars, researchers and professionals. Ellis <i>et al.</i> (1993) realises that <br>information seeking behaviour is comparable and is very similar in different fields, <br> <hr> <A name=4></a>the difference generally comes in the emphasis. There are six main activities <br>identified by Ellis <i>et al.</i> (1993) — starting, chaining, browsing, differentiating, <br>monitoring and extracting. <br> <br><i>Starting</i> <br>In the starting stage of the information seeking process the researcher is beginning a <br>new or unfamiliar project. This initial familiarisation involves “... activities <br>characteristic of the initial search for information” (Ellis <i>et al.</i> 1993, p. 359) and <br>includes obtaining starting references and information. The idea is to identify the <br>topic and begin a search for relevant information. In starting a research project there <br>are many informal and formal resources one could use. Informal resources can <br>include personal contacts or colleagues, browsing through catalogue systems or the <br>Internet. Formal resources are such things as printed indexes, formal bibliographies, <br>research guides, and abstracts. <br> <br>In the field of Science, the most common way of gaining the initial information <br>needed to begin a project is through personal contacts (Ellis <i>et al.</i> 1993; Hallmark <br>1994; Seggern 1995). Ellis <i>et al.</i> (1993) explains that is because there are usually <br>contactable fellow scientists who are familiar with information regarding this new <br>topic, or for those scientists who are doing PhD research, the initial references are <br>usually provided by their supervisors. Another source of starting information for <br>scientists comes from keeping up to date with reviews, prominent authors and articles <br>in fields of interest and knowing where to locate these introductory references (Ellis <i>et <br>al.</i> 1993; Hallmark 1994). Computer scientists also rely heavily on the above <br>informal sources and less on the formal sources. However, computer scientists also <br>include the use of the Internet to view authors’ sites and the World Wide Web (in <br>conjunction with search engines) to locate initial information (Cunningham and <br>Connaway). In the same flavour, social scientists also rely on personal contacts (Ellis <br><i>et al.</i> 1993). However, social scientists also use such formal sources as abstracts and <br>indexes, bibliographies, catalogues and book reviews (Folster 1995; Hartmann 1995). <br>In contrast to scientists, people in the field of humanities tend to use formal resources <br>more. They mainly use printed primary sources, abstracting and indexing sources, <br>catalogues, research guides, and formal bibliographies (Broadbent 1986; Sievert and <br>Sievert 1989; Wiberley and Jones 1989). Non-academic professionals, on the other <br>hand, have a different outlook on the initial resources used. They generally use <br>informal sources, including colleagues, trade publications and unpublished reports <br>(Pinelli 1991; Holland and Powell 1995; Leckie <i>et al.</i> 1996). Leckie <i>et al.</i> (1996) <br>notes that professionals rely more heavily on their personal files, knowledge and <br>experience. “Shuchman (1981) reports that engineers first consult their personal store <br>of technical information, followed in order by informal discussions with colleagues, <br>discussions with supervisors, use of internal technical reports, and contact with a <br>“key” person in the organization who usually knows where the needed information <br>may be located” (Pinelli 1991, p. 19). <br> <br>Nearly all researchers use personal contacts or colleagues for initial information <br>sources, but there is a noticeable difference in the use of formal resources between <br>fields of study. There are two principle factors which determine the use of particular <br>sources for information: accessibility and quality (Ford 1973). Accessibility is based <br>on the perceived cost of attaining the source of information. For example, it could be <br>based on the distance to travel or the time delay waiting to retrieve the resource. <br> <hr> <A name=5></a>Accessibility is seen as one of the strongest predictors of use. Quality “governs the <br>acceptability of the information retrieved” (Ford 1973, p. 88). Studies note that <br>researchers generally do not rely on libraries for providing the information required in <br>the starting phase of the information gathering process (Folster 1995). Libraries or <br>librarians are seen as sources for acquiring material previously identified as relevant, <br>rather than as a primary source for identifying relevant information. They do not play <br>an important part in the initial search process for sources (Folster 1995). However, <br>academics in humanities read, on average, more than people in other fields of study. <br>A consequence of this is that they tend to know where to find information required to <br>start a new project, and generally make more use of the library and its facilities <br>(Wiberley and Jones 1989). <br> <br><i>Chaining</i> <br>The chaining or chasing stage is “...following chains of citations or other forms of <br>referential connection between material” (Ellis <i>et al.</i> 1993, p. 359). Chaining involves <br>locating references to further work by using relevant material already retrieved. Ellis <br><i>et al.</i> (1993) categorises chaining as being either forward or backward chaining. <br>Backward chaining looks at the references within an article to locate other relevant <br>printed articles written in the past. Forward chaining makes use of citation indexes to <br>find out which articles have cited the relevant article you possess (Ellis <i>et al.</i> 1993). <br>Another method of chaining is using catalogue systems to locate work with the same <br>author, subject, topic or classification. <br> <br>Most studies regarding information seeking did not state the way in which <br>information is located once the initial relevant references were found. However, <br>Hallmark (1994) remarks that most scientists use references from their literature to <br>chain both backwards and forwards. It is also seen that they make use of the online <br>databases and library facilities. Ellis <i>et al.</i> (1993) finds that for scientists and social <br>scientists “Backward chaining [is] ... identified as the principle means employed to <br>chase references” and that forward chaining is less widely used and understood. Most <br>scientists know about and utilise citation indexes (generally the <i>Science Citation <br>Index</i>). This is unlike the social scientists Ellis <i>et al.</i> (1993) studied who had very <br>little or no knowledge of citation indexes and did not know of the existence of the <br><i>Social Science Citation Index</i>. Social scientists are more likely to use reference lists <br>in books and journals to locate information sources. They also use CD-ROM and <br>online databases (Hartmann 1995). Computer scientists use reference lists to initiate <br>trials (Cunningham and Connaway). They also make use of on-line keyword search <br>techniques. Individuals in the humanities use bibliographical tracings and subject and <br>publisher’s catalogues (Broadbent 1986; Sievert and Sievert 1989; Wiberley and <br>Jones 1989). In the research on information seeking behaviour of professionals, <br>Pinelli (1991), Holland and Powell (1995) and Leckie <i>et al.</i> (1996), did not indicate <br>how people in professional situations locate further information after gaining initial <br>references. <br> <br>The library services used in the chaining stage of information seeking is limited <br>mainly to online bibliographic and catalogue services. Even then, most of those that <br>acknowledge the use of these facilities prefer, when possible, to use these facilities <br>from the comfort of their own personal computers (Cunningham and Connaway). <br> <br> <hr> <A name=6></a><i>Browsing <br></i>Browsing is a “... planned or unplanned examination of sources, journals, books, or <br>other media in the hope of discovering unspecified new, but useful information” <br>(Apted and Choo 1971, p. 228). It is concerned with searching from where to what <br>rather than from what to where (Chang and Rice 1993). However, is must be noted <br>that there are two main types of browsing, across-document browsing and within-<br>document browsing (Marchionini 1995). Across-document browsing is often <br>identified with card catalogue systems or bookshelves and it is when records or books <br>are surveyed to find items to examine more closely. These items could be on a <br>specific topic or to keep up to date. Within-document browsing is mainly used during <br>the differentiation stage of the search process to determine if the material retrieved is <br>relevant or to gain an overview (this is explored further below). Browsing can be <br>seen as either a specific stage in the information seeking process or an activity carried <br>out during phases of the process; for example, during the starting stage one may <br>browse library bookshelves for initial sources of information. <br> <br>Research into different types, the meaning, and evaluation of browsing is discussed by <br>Apted and Choo (1971). This research also finds that there seems to be a contrast <br>between browsing methods used by people in different disciplines. Scientists, for <br>example, tend to browse current material and make a deliberate attempt to include this <br>activity in their information seeking behaviour. It is usually conducted haphazardly <br>and is mainly for maintaining awareness in the current literature. This point is <br>emphasised by Hallmark (1994) who states that “[m]ost scientists argue that browsing <br>in library and personal journal issues is of critical importance in keeping up with the <br>literature” (Hallmark 1994, p. 203-204). The methods of browsing for scientists <br>include browsing in journals, <i>Current Contents</i>, abstracts, along shelves in the library <br>or in bookshops, and displays at conferences (Ellis <i>et al.</i> 1993). Most computer <br>scientists know the primary journals in their field and browsing them is an activity <br>that is performed regularly. It is also recognised that computer scientists browse their <br>personal bookshelves and use the Internet when looking for information sources <br>(Cunningham and Connaway). In contrast, social scientists rank browsing low down <br>in their information seeking tasks, after reference lists, bibliographies, and reviews for <br>use in locating sources of information (Hartmann 1995). This may be due to the <br>structure of the library for providing a useful browsing environment for social <br>scientists. “[Browsing] ... is an approach to information seeking that is informal and <br>opportunistic and depends heavily on the information environment” (Marchionini <br>1995, p. 100). Because of the many topic areas studied by social scientists the books <br>and journals used are vast and wide spread through out the library, making it difficult <br>to browse all the relevant publications. Thus, since the environment is not ideal for a <br>social scientist, browsing can often be unrewarding. For humanities scholars, as in <br>social sciences, browsing is not ranked highly as an information seeking activity. <br>Sievert and Sievert (1989) remarks that browsing for humanists is not a regular habit <br>and that “only a few, a very few had any pattern of browsing anywhere” (Sievert and <br>Sievert 1989, p. 92). When they do browse, however, it is usually a wider base, using <br>both old and new material and material on almost any topic. It also is seen as a less <br>deliberate act, than that of the sciences. In the studies perused, there is little mention <br>on the browsing behaviour of non-academic professionals. Leckie <i>et al</i>. (1996) say <br>that engineers monitor or browse journals. This is perhaps a characteristic of all non-<br>academic professionals. <br> <br> <hr> <A name=7></a>Browsing can be a rewarding task because “Browsing is a natural and effective <br>approach to many types of information-seeking problems. It is natural because it <br>coordinates human physical, emotive, and cognitive resources in the same way that <br>humans monitor the physical world and search for physical objects. It can be <br>effective because the environment and particularly human-created environments are <br>generally organised and highly redundant” (Marchionini 1995, p. 100). The library is <br>an organised environment classified to invite browsing by topic area, yet there are <br>some disciplines in which their subject can include many topics scattered through out <br>the classification scheme. This may be a reason why most individuals prefer to <br>browse their own collections rather than browse library bookshelves for relevant <br>information. Apted and Choo (1971) lists a few ideas that could improve library <br>browsing, one being the use of small sections of material, continually refreshed with <br>information of high interest and potential. However, there are arguments for and <br>against such structuring (Apted and Choo 1971; Ellis <i>et al.</i> 1993). <br> <br><i>Differentiating</i> <br>Ellis <i>et al.</i> (1993) define differentiating as “... an activity which uses differences <br>between sources as a filter on the nature and quality of the material examined” (Ellis <br><i>et al.</i> 1993, p. 362). Differentiating is based on human judgement to determine the <br>relevance of the information retrieved. Schamber (1994) addresses relevance and the <br>problems surrounding an accurate definition. The term ‘relevance’, when discussed <br>here, pertains to the situational view where it “refers to a relationship between <br>information and the user’s information problem situation” (Schamber 1994, p. 8). <br>The selection of material based on some predefined criteria defines the usefulness or <br>satisfaction of the information retrieved. This criteria can be based on the actual <br>information contained in the publication, or guidelines such as cost saving, precision, <br>completeness, credibility, and convenience of location (Gluck 1996), or it could be <br>based on the perceived relevance of specific authors, journals, institutions, etc. <br> <br>To determine relevance on the basis of subject, individuals often read the material <br>specifically to gain an overview to form an opinion on its content. Browsing can be <br>used to ascertain this. “For example, by scanning the title page, table of contents, <br>section headings, index, and reference list of a book, we gain a sense of the content’s <br>scope, depth of coverage, and the author’s organizational perspective and thereby can <br>decide quickly whether to invest time reading it. It is important to note that in the <br>case of books, those attributes that we browse first are well-established standards to <br>aid browsing” (Marchionini 1995, p. 102). Marchionini (1995) calls this within-<br>document browsing. <br> <br>It was found that research did not specifically comment on selection behaviours of <br>specific disciplines. But the research did reveal that most scholars differentiate <br>between sources of information on the basis of the material’s subject. Ellis <i>et al.</i> <br>(1993) state that scientists and social scientists tend to use factors such as topic, <br>author, and journal source. The source of information was also analysed for the <br>quality, level and type to decide relevance. From these factors, a list of core journals <br>is often determined which can also be used to identify material. Researchers in <br>humanities also have a high regard for authors and also works (Sievert and Sievert <br>1989). As with the other disciplines, studies on professionals did not cover how they <br>determine information relevance. <br> <br> <hr> <A name=8></a>Determining the relevance of a document or source is solely an individual’s <br>perspective, so the library or librarian is not a determining factor in differentiating <br>sources. It would, however, be advantageous for a library to know and have the <br>relevant material available for use. Traditionally, librarians have sort to provide <br>relevant material. However, due to the rising cost of documents, they have had to be <br>more selective in their acquisitions. In response, positions such as special librarians <br>were created. A special librarian is usually engaged to determine and purchase <br>relevant material for an associated subject area (Folster 1995). This service is <br>advantageous to both novices and experts in a particular subject area. These librarians <br>can direct novices to their area of interest, knowing where relevant material might be <br>located. Experts benefit from this system because their subject area has been <br>investigated so that relevant and frequently used information is easily accessed. <br> <br><i>Monitoring</i> <br>Monitoring “is the activity of maintaining awareness of developments in an area <br>through regularly following particular sources” (Ellis <i>et al.</i> 1993, p. 362). As <br>previously noted, a large part of monitoring is conducted using browsing techniques. <br>However, browsing is also a major information gathering technique in its own right. <br>In monitoring the individual must determine a select range of sources to look at so as <br>not to get overwhelmed. These sources are usually the predominant sources used in <br>the particular field. There may be different sources of information used in each <br>discipline for monitoring, but the overall nature and form of the activity is the same <br>(Ellis <i>et al.</i> 1993). <br> <br>For scientists, monitoring often means constantly surveying their small number of <br>core sources, mainly personal contacts and journals. Other sources can include <br>conferences, conference proceedings, magazines, abstracts, books, newspapers, <br>television and computer search updates (Ellis <i>et al.</i> 1993). Scientists also maintain an <br>often concise personal collection of information which is used for monitoring their <br>fields of interest. Seggern (1995) notes that this behaviour is due to the convenience <br>of having the journals on-hand. In comparison, where scientists place a lot of <br>emphasis on journals for maintaining awareness, social scientists use core books and <br>journals equally. They also use newspapers and published catalogues (Ellis <i>et al.</i> <br>1993; Hartmann 1995). Studies on humanists and professionals had no direct <br>references to their monitoring behaviours. Sievert and Sievert (1989) did say that in <br>the humanities they do a lot of reading but they are not concerned with keeping up to <br>date with the most recent publications. Also, Leckie <i>et al.</i> (1996) does say that <br>engineers monitor journals for opportunities and threats and to see “what’s out there”. <br>This may be generalisable to all professionals, but more formal study is required. In <br>an ever changing environment (especially areas like health care and law) professional <br>individuals must keep up with what is happening. <br> <br>From the information gathered on the topic of information seeking patterns of <br>academic disciplines, Folster (1995) concludes that high on the list of priorities for <br>services implemented by libraries should be current awareness services. One such <br>service is <i>Current Contents</i>. <i>Current Contents</i> provides an alternative to scanning <br>journals, but it is not frequently used by academics. Other facilities include printed <br>and electronic abstracts and indexes such as OCLC FirstSearch, UNCOVER, reviews, <br>guides, and citation indexes. <br> <br> <hr> <A name=9></a><i>Extracting</i> <br>Extracting is defined by Ellis <i>et al.</i> (1993) as the behaviour involved in systematically <br>going through a specific source and identifying material to locate or follow up on. <br>Formal sources are more frequently used for systematic analysis, although informal <br>sources may also be used in extracting. This is a task which is primarily carried out <br>during the starting or initial familiarisation phase of the information seeking process <br>to produce a concise list of references to begin searching with. Folster (1995) also <br>includes the reading of material to decide what information will be a part of a final <br>report as an extracting activity. This will be discussed further in a following section <br>on the reading behaviours of people and how they go about extracting information <br>from publications. <br> <br>Research into extracting generally only reveals the sources used. Ellis <i>et al.</i> (1993) is <br>the only study found that discloses the actual use of sources. However, Ellis <i>et al.</i> <br>(1993) only discusses the significance of the activity and reveals the stages of the <br>information seeking process in which extraction of source material is most likely to <br>happen. For most scientists, extracting for further information is a minimal activity <br>that generally only happens in the starting and monitoring stages. In the case of <br>physicists, after initially familiarising oneself with a project, they tend not to seek <br>further. Physicists also tend to use extraction during current awareness activities. <br>Likewise, chemists are inclined to use this activity in writing reviews, forcing them to <br>maintain awareness. The sources used in extracting for scientists are usually journals, <br>monographs, indexes, abstracts, bibliographies and computer databases (Ellis <i>et al.</i> <br>1993). Sources of information that are mainly used by computer scientists are <br>journals and computer databases, specifically the Internet. Online catalogues and CD-<br>ROMs are used infrequently. Computer scientists locate information via the World <br>Wide Web and investigate the home pages of researchers and research institutions. <br>This sort of activity is not an extensive one for computer scientists and they tend to <br>base their own contributions on only one or a few documents. One computer scientist <br>who was surveyed said that “I know people who know the literature too well and <br>never get any research done ... [t]he referees will tell me if I have missed some <br>important reference (Cunningham and Connaway). In a comparison between <br>scientists and social scientists, Ellis <i>et al.</i> (1993) remarks that social scientists use <br>extracting mainly during monitoring. For this group of people, books, journals, book <br>reviews, and bibliographies are the main sources used (Hartmann 1995). Humanities <br>use these sources and also include subject catalogues, printed indexes and research <br>guides (Broadbent 1986; Sievert and Sievert 1989). In studies discussing the <br>information seeking activities of professionals, sources are generally not mentioned. <br>Leckie <i>et al.</i> (1996) notes that professionals make use of trade journals, books, printed <br>catalogues and internal sources. <br> <br>For most users, the library is seen as a reservoir of information, so it is expected to <br>provide easy access to formal sources used to extract information. When extracting is <br>used to maintain current awareness, access is particularly important for browsing and <br>reading the most recent core journals in the respective fields. The material must also <br>be current and relevant. An ideal is again the use of special librarians who know the <br>particular sources which are reliable and applicable for specific fields of study. <br> <br> <hr> <A name=10></a><i>Verification and Ending</i> <br>Verification and ending are information gathering activities used during the verifying <br>and ending phases of researching. In verifying, the information and sources used to <br>produce their own material are checked for information accuracy and errors. The <br>sorts of problems that come to light include typographical, numerical, equation, and <br>citation errors. Verification for most only involves knowing and using reliable <br>sources. To take it further, “one chemist did a spot check on everything, as well as <br>checking obvious errors and material from sources he regarded as unreliable; another <br>did a spot check on new textbooks” (Ellis <i>et al.</i> 1993, p. 365). This sort of activity is <br>seen as minor and is usually subsumed under other activities; for example, social <br>scientists tend to include it under chaining (Ellis <i>et al.</i> 1993). <br> <br>Ending is the assembly and dissemination of information or the actual drawing <br>together of material for publication. It covers the information seeking activities <br>concerned with finishing a topic or project (Ellis <i>et al.</i> 1993; Folster 1995). Most <br>scholars do their major information gathering activities at the start of a project for <br>initial familiarisation, and some also perform literature and information searches <br>during the lifetime of the project. However, Ellis <i>et al.</i> (1993) notes that some <br>chemists returned to the literature at the writing up stage of the project to discuss their <br>contribution in light of the reviewed literature. Two of these chemists minimally <br>collated information in the starting stage of the project and performed a thorough <br>information search at the end. “Both were aware of dangers with this type of <br>approach in finding material at the end which would have led them to modify the <br>work they carried out or in finding that the work had already been undertaken” (Ellis <br><i>et al.</i> 1993, p. 365). <br> <br><i>Location and Delivery of Material</i> <br>In regard to researching behaviour, there are other aspects that need to be considered <br>that are not discussed by Ellis <i>et al.</i> (1993). These are the location and delivery of <br>material and the implications of the decisions made in these areas. Locating a known <br>document or publication reference is often by using an individual’s own collection, <br>the library, or the interlibrary loan system (interloan). <br> <br>The majority of the people surveyed in the articles examined stated that they <br>maintained and extensively used personal collections of journals, documents and/or <br>books. It is not surprising that a personal collection is kept, as the core material <br>related to their field of interest is often known by researchers. Sievert and Sievert <br>(1989) had respondents who “commented that once they [humanists] knew an item <br>was likely to be of importance to them, they tended to purchase it” (Sievert and <br>Sievert 1989, p. 85). A preference for their own collections is mainly due to <br>convenience. For scientists, this reason was mentioned most often by the researchers <br>surveyed (Seggern 1995). This is because they preferred their own classification <br>systems and their own environments. They did not like the barrier experienced in <br>libraries such as temporary unavailability due to binding or use by others. Other <br>reasons for having a personal collection are: researchers can annotate the text for <br>their own purposes (Sievert and Sievert 1989); local libraries no longer carried the <br>essential journals for the researcher’s discipline; the problems with obtaining journals <br>that are now stored in storage due of lack of shelf space; loss, theft, negligence of <br>material; missing and mutilated journals; and general accessibility (Hallmark 1994). <br> <hr> <A name=11></a>It has been found that researchers rely more on the items they have on hand rather <br>than relying on library services (Folster 1995). <br> <br>If researchers do not have the required reference or information in their own <br>collection, they will often resort to using their local library collections. The library is <br>seen as a repository for information and a mechanism for document delivery for those <br>items not owned. Librarians are rarely consulted by researchers when looking for <br>information. Some researchers surveyed commented that the library is a <br>supplementary source rather than a primary source of information. It is seen as a <br>place to get information from once a reference has been found, or a place that <br>provides document delivery services (Ford 1973; Sievert and Sievert 1989; Folster <br>1995). Some find that the library system is pleasing and easy to obtain the necessary <br>material from the shelves, but others lack an appreciation for the library classification <br>system, believing it to be difficult to navigate (Hallmark 1994). <br> <br>Researchers can generally agree on two things, that their local library services are <br>usually adequate for locating material and that they make extensive use of interloan <br>facilities. The material that is not readily available elsewhere can be retrieved via <br>interloan, which is usually done through the local library. The unavailability of <br>material at the local library most often results in an interloan request (Hallmark 1994; <br>Hartmann 1995). However, for computer scientists, interloan was found to be only <br>used when the material could not be located at the library or on the World Wide Web <br>(Cunningham and Connaway). The only problem with interloan is the time delay <br>from the request for information to actually receiving it. Hallmark (1994) concludes <br>that “They [researchers] do expect and need fast, efficient, and inexpensive document <br>delivery for material not owned and not available electronically” (Hallmark 1994, p. <br>208) and says that at present there is an unacceptable wait. It points out that requests <br>that have taken too long are no longer of interest. For professionals, at least, it seems <br>that accessibility is a major issue when requiring information. Pinelli (1991) and <br>Leckie <i>et al.</i> (1996) state that accessibility appears to be a criteria used most often <br>when selecting an information source even if that source proved to be the least useful <br>or not of high quality. Another issue that is stipulated is timeliness. Information that <br>can be obtained immediately or in a reasonable amount of time is more likely to be <br>used. The usefulness and impact of the retrieved information will decrease as time <br>proceeds (Leckie <i>et al.</i> 1996). So, the relevance of a document is often based on <br>accessibility and timeliness, two of the down sides to using interloan. “[The biggest <br>problem] is being able to obtain the article easily and rapidly...in such cases <br>interlibrary loan can be too slow and require too much time and effort to be <br>worthwhile” (Hallmark 1994, p. 206). <br> <br><i>Summary</i> <br>From looking at how researchers in the academic and professional roles conduct <br>information seeking and retrieval, it is interesting to note that the library is mostly <br>used as a source for previously identified material, to browse bookshelves (mainly for <br>current awareness), and for the interloan facilities. This definition of library usage is <br>very different from what libraries provide and researchers are recommended to use. <br>To further strengthen the argument, Folster (1995) suggests that improvements to <br>services mean that libraries must focus on document delivery services, current <br>awareness services, and customised search services, as these are the most utilised <br>facilities. The article also advises training in new technologies. <br> <hr> <A name=12></a> <br>In most cases, the way in which researchers of different disciplines conduct <br>information seeking and retrieval is very similar. Often the difference between <br>disciplines is in the sources used and the importance attached to the activity. The <br>actual act is the same across the fields. When looking at the differences in the use of <br>libraries by researchers, they are significant. Humanists and social scientists boast <br>that they use the library a lot more frequently than scientists and computer scientists. <br>Professionals, on the other hand, use the library rarely. Most people overall may use <br>the library to retrieve information at some time, but a lot do not know about or use <br>other facilities offered by the library. Holland and Powell (1995) describes a survey <br>performed on a sample of engineers who took a specific course at university. This <br>course involved formal training on conducting information research. The responses to <br>that survey and to a survey conducted on another sample engineers, who did not take <br>this course, were compared. A result of the comparison was that both groups of <br>people showed similar information gathering preferences, but those that took the <br>above course showed more awareness of library services. Increasing the exposure of <br>the library leads to an increase in the use of materials and services (Ford 1973). <br>These trends discussed here are replicated in Broadbent (1986), where it is noted that <br>inexperience is the cause of the limited knowledge of library services. This lack of <br>formal training among researchers is not uncommon; for example, all the computer <br>scientists in Cunningham and Connaway had received no instruction in conducting a <br>literature search or in using the common indices. The result of this is that users of the <br>library do not make the most of services available to them, and the library is not seen <br>as anything more than an information repository. <br> <br><b>Information Use and Storage <br></b> <br>Once relevant material has been located and retrieved, information is then extracted <br>for use. How individuals read can be analysed for insight into their behaviour during <br>this activity. Most research into reading concentrates on either identifying letters, <br>words, and sentences when learning to read, or on the cognitive processes involved, or <br>on strategies for reading better or more efficiently. There is very little documented <br>research found on how readers actually behave when confronted with material — <br>where and when reading occurs, what is read, and how information is extracted from <br>relevant material. Research on the utilisation of materials — the what-where-when-<br>how-and-why of material use — in the library yields similar results due to the <br>difficulty to record such activities (Ford 1973). <br> <br><i>Reading Environment</i> <br>An integral part of reading behaviour is the effect of the environment on the reader. <br>The environment can influence concentration and reading ability. Preferences for <br>reading environments are subject to the self-defined factors of users. Factors for <br>choosing a particular reading area can include noise or distractions (or the lack there <br>of), the presence of other people, privacy, seating arrangements, and the availability <br>of other materials (Sommer 1966; Gifford and Sommer 1968; Sommer 1968; Fishman <br>and Walitt 1972). An assumption cannot be made that there is one optimal reading <br>environment that will meet the needs of all individuals (Gifford and Sommer 1968; <br>Sommer 1968) so it is recommended that in designing reading areas, there needs to be <br>a variety of reading spaces for everyone. In this way individuals can choose the most <br>suitable place according to their reading preferences. <br> <hr> <A name=13></a> <br>Reading, primarily for research, can be done in such places as the library, in study <br>rooms, offices, etc. Most research is inclined to look at the library when discussing <br>reading environments. Contradicting this, most studies reviewed indicated that <br>researcher do not spend much time in the library. Thus, there is a requirement to <br>know what researchers do and need in the place that they actually read. However, <br>Ford (1973) looks at the study environment in the library. The requirements of library <br>patrons include personal needs such as “[c]onditions of work — heating, lighting, <br>draughts, sound-proofing, ease of entry/exit — turnstiles, porters etc. ... [a]menities — <br>location of lavatories, smoking rooms, food, drink” (Ford 1973, p. 88). It is also <br>noted that these and other needs of people are variable and that it is important for the <br>library to cater for all. <br> <br>The reading environment also includes how material is presented. This affects not <br>only the readability but can also influence the processing of information. Duchastel <br>(1982) investigates text processing and finds that the presentation of material is <br>particular to its purpose. For example, “[d]ictionaries ... are used for looking up the <br>meaning of words, reference books for finding out specific information about a <br>subject, novels for entertainment [and] ... [t]extbooks are used primarily for learning” <br>(Duchastel 1982, p. 170). The use of these materials are considerably different and so <br>the display techniques to aid information processing is therefore specific to the <br>material’s purpose. Such techniques include labelling, highlighting and illustrating. <br> <br>The presentation medium of reading material is generally either printed or electronic. <br>There is a lot of research into the effects of reading from a screen and comparing this <br>to reading from a paper copy (Askwall 1985; Mills and Weldon 1987; Oborne and <br>Holton 1988; Muter and Maurutto 1991; De Bruijn <i>et al.</i> 1992). It has been found <br>that today, due to computers being more advanced, comparatively faster, and more <br>reliable, and owing to increased exposure to the computing environment, there is little <br>evidence that there is a difference in reading speed or comprehension when material is <br>presented on a screen or hard copy (Askwall 1985; Oborne and Holton 1988; Muter <br>and Maurutto 1991). Therefore, if these are the only two factors considered of <br>significance when determining readability, then the reading medium is dependent on <br>the readers preference. However, reading and comprehension are only two of many <br>factors which dictate the use of paper versus computers when reading (Oborne and <br>Holton 1988). <br> <br>Paper copies are widely used because paper is permanent; it can be recalled without <br>recourse to high technology; it is convenient; and easily transportable (Showstack <br>1982; Oborne and Holton 1988). “Paper ... is still the most popular method of <br>communications and is likely to remain so” (Plume 1988 quoted in Muter 1991, p. <br>257). In comparison to computer screens, paper appears to be easier and faster to <br>read, but the size of the effect depends on the quality of both the paper and the screen <br>presentation (Mills and Weldon 1987). Use of computer screens for reading <br>electronic copies is often dependent on the textual display. Advances in technology <br>have increased the legibility of computer screens through better resolution, clearer and <br>more varied fonts, negative contrast capability (dark characters on a light background) <br>and a higher refresh rate, to name a few. This provides flexibility in textual <br>presentation of information (Muter and Maurutto 1991). Merrill (1982), Mills and <br>Weldon (1987), and Muter and Maurutto (1991) list several factors concerning how to <br> <hr> <A name=14></a>display information on computer screens to get optimum readability. Readers may <br>prefer computer screens, even though users’ performance may not be as good with <br>computer screens as with paper (Mills and Weldon 1987). Both paper and electronic <br>copies have their advantages and disadvantages. Perhaps readability is dependent on <br>reader preference where the reader determines which of the mediums disadvantages <br>them the least. <br> <br>When looking at researchers who were surveyed, there are mixed responses to their <br>preference for reading medium. Computer scientists preferred to use paper copies. <br>Documents that were retrieved in electronic format were printed and in most cases <br>only the printed documents were retained. The electronic copy might be kept while it <br>was of immediate use (Cunningham and Connaway). In contrast, Holland and Powell <br>(1995) found that engineers preferred to receive information in electronic form and <br>would prefer to receive less paper in the future. Scientists also expressed the <br>usefulness of the retrieval of full text online documents, then “files could then be <br>viewed on the scientist’s screen and printed on the local laser printed if desired <br>(Hallmark 1994, p. 206). These preferences for electronic documents were mainly <br>due to the convenience of retrieval. Research was not found to confirm that these <br>electronic documents were then read online. <br> <br><i>Reading for Information Use <br></i>“In simple terms, information has only one use — ie. the assistance of problem <br>solving” (Ford 1973, p. 88). One main technique for extracting information is by <br>reading. Alternative techniques are listening and viewing an oral discussion, <br>presentation, demonstration, etc. When looking at reading as an activity for <br>extracting information to use, there are positives and negatives associated with <br>reading as an activity for extracting (Norman 1993). Positive aspects of reading are <br>that the individual has control over which portion of text is read, which is skipped, <br>which is repeated, and at any moment they can stop reading. Moreover, it gives them <br>the chance to reflect on what has been read, so that they can contemplate, question, <br>ponder and agree or disagree. On the other side, reading can be comparatively slow <br>and difficult in comparison to other mediums of information. It takes training and <br>practice and “[r]eading ... requires relatively greater effort and thought” (Showstack <br>1982, p371). Reading takes mental effort, mental demands, concentration, and <br>requires a focus of attention on the material. “Written material tends to be <br>information-rich, so that considerable mental activity is needed to decode the author’s <br>message” (Norman 1993, p. 244). <br> <br>There are many research and study guides that instruct readers on the benefits of using <br>reading methods to increase reading speed and comprehension. One such strategy for <br>scanning documents to determine the material of relevance is recorded in Booth <i>et al.</i> <br>(1995) where speedier reading is achieved through five steps. Booth <i>et al.</i> (1995) <br>acknowledges that this is only used to identify and understand the work and states that <br>the important sources require careful reading. More thorough strategies for critical <br>reading was developed by Hardcastle (1996) from a variety of sources. This proposes <br>that these strategies can make reading more satisfying and productive. The seven <br>strategies include: previewing, learning about a text before actually reading it; <br>contextualising, placing a text in its historical, biographical, and cultural context; <br>questioning to understand and remember, asking questions about the content; <br>reflecting on challenges to your beliefs and values, examining you personal responses; <br> <hr> <A name=15></a>outlining and summarising, identifying the main ideas and restating them in your own <br>words; evaluating an argument; testing the logic of a text as well as its credibility and <br>emotional impact; and comparing and contrasting related readings, exploring <br>likenesses and differences between texts to understand them better (Hardcastle 1996). <br>Another reading method is examined in Sweet <i>et al.</i> (1993) for use within a teaching <br>and learning environment. These reading strategies can be used to extract information, <br>but in doing so ideas, arguments and conclusions are also formed. <br> <br>Strategic reading usually insists on the use of annotations and notes to guide <br>information extraction. Hardcastle’s (1996) critical reading strategies, reviewed <br>above, state that annotating directly on the page is fundamental to these techniques. <br>Annotations can include, “underlining key words, phrases, or sentences; writing <br>comments or questions in the margins; bracketing important sections of the text; <br>constructing ideas with lines or arrows; numbering related points in sequence; and <br>making note of anything that strikes you as interesting, important, or questionable” <br>(Hardcastle 1996). Annotations are usually written directly on a paper copy to refer <br>directly to specific parts of the text for reading clarity, proof-reading, or refereeing. <br>“[U]sers often show a strong preference for the “hard-copy” medium of document <br>presentation when it comes to reading activities such as those that involve proof-<br>reading or refereeing the document” (Tucker and Jones 1993), even though the use of <br>computers for displaying documents is increasing. When using electronic copies of <br>documents, some editors provide annotating facilities. These can be awkward to use, <br>often requiring specific file formats and dictating how the user must annotate. This is <br>discussed further by Tucker and Jones (1993) with respect to the use of written, typed <br>or spoken annotations. Annotating is an individual task that enhances and establishes <br>the author’s message as the reader interprets it. Differences in annotation marks when <br>comparing the same document that has been annotated by two people are linked to <br>personal understanding and preferences (Showstack 1982) and it has been found that <br>“[m]ost readers annotate in layers, adding further annotations on second and third <br>readings. Annotations can be light or heavy, depending on the reader’s purpose and <br>the difficulty of the material” (Hardcastle 1996). <br> <br>It is recommended that in addition to annotating that the reader should take notes to <br>reflect the “quality of thinking” at the time (Booth <i>et al.</i> 1995). Specific information <br>that should be recorded include, bibliographical data, key words, summaries and <br>thoughts, and a call number (if applicable). Making notes can enhance reading by <br>focussing the reader’s concentration, increasing the reader’s understanding of the text, <br>and enforcing an evaluation of the quality of the source document. It is used to gather <br>information and create links between what you know and what you have read from <br>different sources. This gives you a broader perspective and the ability to draw <br>conclusions. It also records information that can be stored for later use (LDC 1996). <br> <br>Although the strategies above dictate how one <i>should</i> read, it is not known how <br>individuals <i>actually</i> read. In particular, frequency and quality are characteristics of <br>reading which are often unknown and hard to measure. Reading for information can <br>be a specific, casual or a subliminal activity. With the amount of information <br>propelled upon individuals, how much does a person read and process? One way of <br>measuring reading frequency for researchers is by determining the utilisation of <br>library materials. It has been found that even though the materials that are on loan are <br>often retained until the due date, the use made of the materials is small relative to the <br> <hr> <A name=16></a>length of time they are on loan (Ford 1973). The quality of use during this period is <br>not known. By looking at the time allocation of researchers to the activity of reading, <br>one may be able to determine not only how often reading occurs, but the quality and <br>quantity of information read. <br> <br><i>Information Storage <br></i>During the research period and afterwards it would be of interest to know what <br>happens to the material, information, and notes collected? Also, what sort of format <br>are they kept in? These questions may be answered by looking at the preferences for <br>reading environments. Examples include, a partiality for paper or electronic copy, the <br>original or a photocopy, borrowed or own copy of material. In general, from the <br>research seen, most scholars prefer to have their own hard copy (be it the original or a <br>photocopy). Most, if not all, information is retained and filed for possible use the <br>future. It has already been noted that researchers prefer their own collections; one <br>reason given by scientists is that they then can apply their own classification systems <br>when filing materials (Seggern 1995). Such schemes are personalised and can be <br>quite elaborate. Sievert and Sievert (1989) describes the organisation of research <br>materials by the humanists surveyed. Most scholars have some sort of informal <br>classification scheme for filing which may include index cards, notebooks or some <br>combination of books, photocopied articles or a log of some sort. Documents can be <br>arranged by the traditional Dewy Decimal, alphanumeric, or Library of Congress <br>classification numbers, or more likely grouped by author or subject. Cunningham and <br>Connaway captures the essence of scholarly filing: “piles, generally not by project ... <br>boxes of reprints, folders of notes, folders in the filing cabinet, pigeonholes with <br>papers and drafts for recent papers ... basically it’s chaos. Usually I manage to find <br>the most important stuff” (Cunningham and Connaway). <br> <br><i>Summary <br></i>Ford (1973) gives a good summation of reading behaviour in libraries in that there is a <br>large gap in our knowledge when it comes to the utilisation of materials. One can <br>only assume that the accuracy of the reading strategies available has been investigated <br>as to their worth and practicality and that they are characteristic of reading behaviour. <br>These strategies and directions for reading give an overall impression on how readers <br>should read. Most readers obviously apply some or all of the guidelines. <br>Consequently, all of these strategies can be included as reader behaviour, although <br>some are more characteristic than others. <br> <br><b>Conclusion <br></b> <br>By gaining a fuller understanding of how users of traditional libraries behave and the <br>reading styles of individuals, the knowledge can be applied to enhance a digital <br>library environment. It can be used to create an inviting and familiar surrounding that <br>caters for the majority of users. Research into library-user interaction is quite <br>thorough in contrast to the research on the behaviour involved in information use and <br>storage. Ford (1973) discovered a gap in knowledge on the what-where-when-how-<br>and-why of material use in libraries. There is also a lack of information on the actual <br>reading methods used, the frequency and quality of material use, and the storage of <br>documents. Further research is required to extend our knowledge on these <br>behaviours. In light of the research found on traditional library interactions, it would <br>be interesting to know if the patrons of this library environment have the same <br> <hr> <A name=17></a>requirements as the digital library patrons. It would be beneficial for designers of <br>digital libraries to understand the behaviours of the people they are intended for. <br> <br><b>References <br></b> <br>Apted, E., and Choo, C. W. (1971). General purposive browsing. <i>Library Association </i><br> <i>Record</i>, <b>73</b>, 228-230. <br> <br>Askwall, S. (1985). Computer supported reading vs reading text on paper: a <br> comparison of two reading situations. <i>International Journal of Man-Machine <br>Studies</i>, <b>22</b>, 425-439. <br> <br>Booth, W. C., Colomb, G. G., and Williams, J. M., (1995). <i>The craft of research</i>. <br> University of Chicago. <br> <br>Broadbent, E. (1986). A study of humanities faculty library information seeking <br> behaviour. <i>Cataloging &amp; Classification Quarterly</i>, <b>6</b>, 23-37. <br> <br>Bystrom, K., and Jarvelin, K. (1995). Task complexity affects information seeking <br> and use. <i>Information Processing &amp; Management</i>, <b>13</b>, 191-213. <br> <br>Chang, S. J., and Rice R. E. (1993). Browsing: a multi-dimensional framework. <br> <i>Annual Review of Information Science and Technology</i>, <b>28</b>, 231-276. <br> <br>Cunningham, S. J., and Connaway, L. S. (). Information searching preferences and <br> practices of computer science researchers. Working paper. <br> <br>De Bruijn, D., De Mul, S., and Van Oostendorp, H. (1992). The influence of screen <br> size and text layout on the study of text. <i>Behaviour &amp; Information Technology</i>, <br><b>11</b>, 71-78. <br> <br>Duchastel, P. C. (1982). Textual display techniques. In <i>The technology of text</i>, (ed. D. <br> H. Jonassen), pp. 167-192. Educational Technology Publications, New Jersey. <br> <br>Ellis, D., Cox, D., and Hall, K. (1993). A comparison of the information seeking <br> patterns of researchers in the physical and social sciences. <i>Journal of <br>Documentation</i>, <b>49</b>, 356-369. <br> <br>Fishman, D., and Walitt, R. (1972). Seating and area preferences in a college reserve <br> room. <i>College &amp; Research Libraries</i>, <b> 33</b>, 284-297. <br> <br>Folster, M. B. (1995). Information seeking patterns: social sciences. In <i>Library users </i><br> <i>and reference services</i>, (ed. J. B. Whitlatch), pp. 83-93. <br> <br>Ford, G. (1973). Progress in documentation: research in user behaviour in university <br> libraries. <i>Journal of Documentation</i>, <b>29</b>, 85-106. <br> <br>Gifford, R., and Sommer, R. (1968). The desk or the bed? <i>Personnel &amp; Guidance </i><br> <i>Journal</i>, <b>46</b>, 876-878. <br> <br> <hr> <A name=18></a>Gluck, M. (1996). Exploring the relationship between user satisfaction and relevance <br> in information systems. <i>Information Processing &amp; Management</i>, <b>32</b>, 89-104. <br> <br>Hallmark, J. (1994). Scientists’ access and retrieval of references cited in their recent <br> journal articles. <i>College &amp; Research Libraries</i>, <b>55</b>, 199-209. <br> <br>Hardcastle, V. (1996). <i>Critical reading strategies</i>. Found at <br> http://mind.phil.vt.edu/www/1204.html, on 6 December 1996. <br> <br>Hartmann, J. (1995). Information needs of anthropologists. <i>Behavioural &amp; Social </i><br> <i>Sciences Librarian</i>, <b>13</b>, 13-24. <br> <br>Holland, M. P., and Powell, C. K. (1995). A longitudinal survey of the information <br> seeking and use habits of some engineers. <i>College and Research Libraries</i>, <b>56</b>, 7-<br>15. <br> <br>Kuhlthau, C. C. (1993). A principle of uncertainty for information seeking. <i>Journal of </i><br> <i>Documentation</i>, <b>49</b>, 339-355. <br> <br>LDC (Learning and Development Centre). (1996). <i>Reading and Notemaking</i>. Found <br> at http://www.macarther.uws.edu.au/ssd/ldc/Readnote.html, on 21 January 1997. <br> <br>Leckie, G. L., Pettigrew, K. E., and Sylvain, C. (1996). Modeling the information <br> seeking of professionals: a general model derived from research on engineers, <br>health professionals, and lawyers. <i>The Library Quarterly</i>, <b>66</b>, 161-193. <br> <br>Marchionini, G. (1995). <i>Information seeking in electronic environments</i>. Cambridge <br> University Press, New York. <br> <br>Merrill, P. F. (1982). Displaying text on microcomputers. In <i>The technology of text</i>, <br> (ed. D. H. Jonassen), pp. 401-414. Educational Technology Publications, New <br>Jersey. <br> <br>Mills, C. B., and Weldon L. J. (1987). Reading text from computer screens. <i>ACM </i><br> <i>Computing Surveys</i>, <b>19</b>, 329-358. <br> <br>Muter, P., and Maurutto, P. (1991). Reading and skimming from computer screens <br> and books: the paperless office revisited? <i>Behaviour &amp; Information Technology</i>, <br><b>10</b>, 257-266. <br> <br>Norman, D. A. (1993). <i>Things that make us smart: defending human attributes in the </i><br> <i>age of the machine</i>. Addison-Wesley, Massachusetts. <br> <br>Oborne, D. J., and Holton, D. (1988). Reading from screen versus paper: there is no <br> difference. <i>International Journal of Man-Machine Studies</i>, <b>28</b>, 1-9. <br> <br>Pinelli, T. E. (1991). The information-seeking habits and practices of engineers. <br> <i>Science and Technology Libraries</i>, <b>12</b>, 5-16. <br> <br> <hr> <A name=19></a>Schamber, L. (1994). Relevance and information behavior. <i>Annual Review of </i><br> <i>Information Science and Technology</i>, <b>29</b>, 3-48. <br> <br>Seggern, M. V. (1995). Scientists, information seeking and reference services. In <br> <i>Library users and reference services</i>, (ed. J. B. Whitlatch), pp. 95-104. <br> <br>Showstack, R. (1982). Printing: the next stage: discourse punctuation. In <i>The </i><br> <i>technology of text</i>, (ed. D. H. Jonassen), pp. 369-376. Educational Technology <br>Publications, New Jersey. <br> <br>Sievert, D., and Sievert, M. E. (1989). Philosophical research: report from the field. <br> <i>Proceedings of the Humanists at Work symposium</i> (April, Chicago), pp. 79-94. <br>University of Illinois at Chicago. <br> <br>Sommer, R. (1966). The ecology of privacy. <i>Library Quarterly</i>, <b>36</b>, 234-248. <br> <br>Sommer, R. (1968). Reading ares in college libraries. <i>Library Quarterly</i>, <b>38</b>, 249-260. <br> <br>Sweet, A. P., Riley, R. W., Robinson, S. P., and Conaty, J. C. (1993). <i>State of the art: </i><br> <i>transforming ideas for teaching and learning to read</i>. Found at <br>http://www.ed.gov/pubs/StateArt/covpg.html, on 21 January 1997.<i> </i><br> <br>Tucker, P., and Jones, D. M. (1993). Document annotation: to write, type or speak? <br> <i>International Journal of Man-Machine Studies</i>, <b>39</b>, 885-900. <br> <br>Wiberley, S., and Jones, W. G. (1989). Patterns of information seeking in the <br> humanities. <i>College &amp; Research Libraries</i>, <b>50</b>, 638-645. <br> <br>Witten, I. H., Cunningham, S. J., Vallabh, M., and Bell, T. C. (1995). A New Zealand <br> digital library for computer science research. <i>Proc Digital Libraries ’95</i> (April, <br>Texas), pp. 25-30. <br> <br> <br> <hr> <A name=20></a><b>Bibliography <br></b> <br>Apted, E., and Choo, C. W. (1971). General purposive browsing. <i>Library Association </i><br> <i>Record</i>, <b>73</b>, 228-230. <br>A good overall article on across-document browsing. It researches different types, <br>the meaning of, and evaluating browsing. It also looks at general browsing in the <br>library environment. Light and easy reading. <br> <br>Arnold, K. (1994). <i>The electronic librarian is a verb / the electronic library is not a </i><br> <i>sentence</i>. Found at http://www.press.umich.edu/jep/works/arnold.eleclib.html, on <br>9 December 1996. <br>A presentation to a conference on the place of libraries and librarians in today’s <br>information society. The author also discusses the challenges of the library in an <br>electronic, user-pays system. Light conversational reading. <br> <br>Askwall, S. (1985). Computer supported reading vs reading text on paper: a <br> comparison of two reading situations. <i>International Journal of Man-Machine <br>Studies</i>, <b>22</b>, 425-439. <br>Compares the use of computers and paper for reading, skimming and <br>comprehension. Contains a good set of references. <br> <br>Booth, W. C., Colomb, G. G., and Williams, J. M., (1995). <i>The craft of research</i>. <br> University of Chicago. <br>Contains strategies that can be used when reading for information. The methods <br>of research discussed are practical and realistic. A very good book to use for <br>researching and writing papers. <br> <br>Broadbent, E. (1986). A study of humanities faculty library information seeking <br> behaviour. <i>Cataloging &amp; Classification Quarterly</i>, <b>6</b>, 23-37. <br>An insight into what library sources humanities faculty use. It has views on the <br>kind of information catalogue facilities they would prefer. It also looks at how <br>faculty prefer to search, indicating electronic services required to cater for this. <br> <br>Bystrom, K., and Jarvelin, K. (1995). Task complexity affects information seeking <br> and use. <i>Information Processing &amp; Management</i>, <b>13</b>, 191-213. <br>The article argues that task alone is not specific enough to analyse information <br>seeking and use behaviour. It provides an information seeking model and a <br>method to look at how the level of task complexity affects the information needs, <br>seeking, channels, and sources. <br> <br>Chang, S. J., and Rice R. E. (1993). Browsing: a multi-dimensional framework. <br> <i>Annual Review of Information Science and Technology</i>, <b>28</b>, 231-276. <br>A good review of the literature published on browsing. This article has a specific <br>section on browsing in a library. Easy to understand. <br> <br>Cunningham, S. J., and Connaway, L. S. (). Information searching preferences and <br> practices of computer science researchers. Working paper. <br>Looks at how and where, under research situations, computer scientists locate <br>information. It covers the information seeking process from seeking to the <br>storage of relevant documents. <br> <hr> <A name=21></a> <br>De Bruijn, D., De Mul, S., and Van Oostendorp, H. (1992). The influence of screen <br> size and text layout on the study of text. <i>Behaviour &amp; Information Technology</i>, <br><b>11</b>, 71-78. <br>This article looks at how changes to the presentation of text on a screen can <br>influence the readability. Two factors discussed is the screen size and text layout. <br>It does not directly look at the reading behaviours of the computer users. <br> <br>Duchastel, P. C. (1982). Textual display techniques. In <i>The technology of text</i>, (ed. D. <br> H. Jonassen), pp. 167-192. Educational Technology Publications, New Jersey. <br>Looks at the display of text in books and how this affects or enhances readability. <br>It offers techniques that can be used to create a more attractive product. This <br>chapter of the book includes a section on how humans process text. <br> <br>Ellis, D., Cox, D., and Hall, K. (1993). A comparison of the information seeking <br> patterns of researchers in the physical and social sciences. <i>Journal of <br>Documentation</i>, <b>49</b>, 356-369. <br>Presents a comparison between physical, chemical and social scientists in the way <br>they go about searching for information. The article develops a very good <br>general model for the information seeking process (it does not specifically relate <br>the activities to the library environment). Thorough and worth while reading. <br> <br>England, M., Shaffer, M. (1995). <i>Librarians in the digital library</i>. Found at <br> http://www.csdl.tamu.edu/DL94/poistion/england.html, on 9 December 1996. <br>This article discusses the roles that a librarian has an opportunity to fulfil in an <br>electronic library environment. It discusses the librarians role as a researcher, <br>organiser and publisher, member of the digital library design team, and as a <br>teacher and consultant. <br> <br>Fishman, D., and Walitt, R. (1972). Seating and area preferences in a college reserve <br> room. <i>College &amp; Research Libraries</i>, <b> 33</b>, 284-297. <br>A study into a specific reading area of one library. It discusses factors of seating <br>preferences (front versus back, the seating position, etc.) and their relevance to <br>users of a college reserve room. The research does not extend to the reading <br>behaviours during the users stay. <br> <br>Folster, M. B. (1995). Information seeking patterns: social sciences. In <i>Library users </i><br> <i>and reference services</i>, (ed. J. B. Whitlatch), pp. 83-93. <br>A look at the information seeking behaviour of social scientists over the last three <br>decades determining trends that library services can be based on. Not very <br>thorough; it covered everything briefly and did not draw many conclusions from <br>the findings. <br> <br>Ford, G. (1973). Progress in documentation: research in user behaviour in university <br> libraries. <i>Journal of Documentation</i>, <b>29</b>, 85-106. <br>A practical look into understanding the interaction between the library and its <br>users and how these findings can be applied to increase library usage. Very <br>relevant and worth while reading. <br> <br> <hr> <A name=22></a>Furuta, K. (1995). <i>Librarianship in the digital library</i>. Found at <br> http://www.csdl.tamu.edu/DL94/position/kfuruta.html, on 9 December 1996. <br>Looks at the role of the librarian in the traditional library and what they can offer <br>a digital library. Also what experience the librarian has that can be used in <br>designing digital libraries. <br> <br>Gessesse, K. (1994). Science communication, electronic access and documentation <br> delivery: the new challenge to the science/engineering reference librarian. <br><i>International Information &amp;Library Review</i>, <b>26</b>, 341-349. <br>Describes the relevance of technology in the information seeking and retrieval <br>process and is a call to reference librarians to embrace it. It looks briefly at the <br>information needs and seeking behaviour of scientists then describes how the <br>reference librarian can assist the scientist in obtaining the material. <br> <br>Gifford, R., and Sommer, R. (1968). The desk or the bed? <i>Personnel &amp; Guidance </i><br> <i>Journal</i>, <b>46</b>, 876-878. <br>Looks at the preferences of study environments for students and whether there is <br>any substantial evidence confirming students study better at a desk. Does not <br>discuss reading behaviour, but does look at environment preferences. <br> <br>Gluck, M. (1996). Exploring the relationship between user satisfaction and relevance <br> in information systems. <i>Information Processing &amp; Management</i>, <b>32</b>, 89-104. <br>Discusses the relevance and user-satisfaction in regard to determining and <br>comparing qualities for information systems. Good definitions and reference to <br>other material regarding relevance and information behaviour. <br> <br>Grogan, D. (1992). <i>Practical reference work</i>. Library Association Publishing, <br> London. <br>Looks at the role of reference librarians in answering library-user enquires <br>sufficiently and accurately. It gives examples of the sort of queries that might be <br>asked by library users. <br> <br>Hallmark, J. (1994). Scientists’ access and retrieval of references cited in their recent <br> journal articles. <i>College &amp; Research Libraries</i>, <b>55</b>, 199-209. <br>A good concise article which discusses how scientists from different fields of <br>interest become aware of relevant information and how they eventually retrieve <br>it. It is opinionated as it draws on the views of the scientists studied indicating <br>their positive and negative perspectives on literature seeking and retrieval. The <br>article discusses the pitfalls of library interaction. <br> <br>Hardcastle, V. (1996). <i>Critical reading strategies</i>. Found at <br> http://mind.phil.vt.edu/www/1204.html, on 6 December 1996. <br>Introduces reading strategies to better understand the material being read. It is a <br>concoction of ideas “culled over the years from various and sundry sources”. <br>Worthwhile looking at when considering reading behaviours. <br> <br>Hartmann, J. (1995). Information needs of anthropologists. <i>Behavioural &amp; Social </i><br> <i>Sciences Librarian</i>, <b>13</b>, 13-24. <br>Covers the information sources and resources of anthropologists in comparison <br> <hr> <A name=23></a>with other social scientists. It looks at the library use and attitudes of <br>anthropologists. <br> <br>Holland, M. P., and Powell, C. K. (1995). A longitudinal survey of the information <br> seeking and use habits of some engineers. <i>College and Research Libraries</i>, <b>56</b>, 7-<br>15. <br>A comparison of graduated engineers who had and had not taken a paper which <br>included formal training on conducting information research. This study shows <br>that those who had done the paper were more aware of library resources. <br> <br>Kuhlthau, C. C. (1991). Inside the search process: information seeking from the user’s <br> perspective. <i>Journal of the American Society for Information Science</i>, <b>42</b>, 361-<br>371. <br>A theoretical approach to information seeking practices. It discusses the <br>behaviour involved in information seeking and builds a model based on past <br>research and a survey conducted. <br> <br>Kuhlthau, C. C. (1993). A principle of uncertainty for information seeking. <i>Journal of </i><br> <i>Documentation</i>, <b>49</b>, 339-355. <br>Develops a high level model of feelings, thoughts, and actions associated with <br>information seeking. Does not look at the physical activities involved or the <br>library. <br> <br>LDC (Learning and Development Centre). (1996). <i>Reading and Notemaking</i>. Found <br> at http://www.macarther.uws.edu.au/ssd/ldc/Readnote.html, on 21 January 1997. <br>Strategies on how to read to gain a better understanding of the material and how <br>making notes can assist this process. A good practical article that is aimed at <br>students. <br> <br>Leckie, G. L., Pettigrew, K. E., and Sylvain, C. (1996). Modeling the information <br> seeking of professionals: a general model derived from research on engineers, <br>health professionals, and lawyers. <i>The Library Quarterly</i>, <b>66</b>, 161-193. <br>A literature review of the needs, uses, location and relevance of information for <br>professions in the fields of engineering, health services and law. An overall <br>model for all professionals, based on this review, is developed and justified. Not <br>a lot of information on the actual search process undertaken by professionals is <br>conveyed. Quite vague. <br> <br>Marchionini, G. (1992). Interfaces for end-user information seeking. <i>Journal of the </i><br> <i>American Society for Information Science</i>, <b>43</b>, 156-163. <br>The process for information seeking is explained and then the best way to solve it <br>via electronic medium is discussed. It covers from defining a problem through to <br>the extraction of information. <br> <br>Marchionini, G. (1995). <i>Information seeking in electronic environments</i>. Cambridge <br> University Press, New York. <br>Looks at the information seeking process in conventional and electronic <br>environments. It includes a very good section on browsing and has a complete <br>set of references. <br> <br> <hr> <A name=24></a>Marchionini, G. (1996). <i>ASIS presentation on browsing</i>. Found at <br> http://www.glue.umd.edu/~march/tea1.html, on 7 January 1996. <br>Slides used by the author to present information on browsing to a conference. <br>The information presented is also found in Marchionini (1995).<i> </i><br> <br>Marchionini, G., Dwiggins, S. S., and Katz, A. (1993). Information seeking in full-<br> text end-user-oriented search systems: the roles of domain and search expertise. <i> <br>Library and Information Science Research</i>, <b>15</b>, 35-69. <br>A look at electronic settings for information searching and compares experts in <br>the domain (for example, scholars) and experts in searching (for example, <br>librarians). Does not discuss the library interaction. <br> <br>Merrill, P. F. (1982). Displaying text on microcomputers. In <i>The technology of text</i>, <br> (ed. D. H. Jonassen), pp. 401-414. Educational Technology Publications, New <br>Jersey. <br>This chapter of the book looks at how text can be displayed on a computer screen <br>that will benefit the reader. It includes a good summary section on the principle <br>of displaying text on a computer. <br> <br>Mills, C. B., and Weldon L. J. (1987). Reading text from computer screens. <i>ACM </i><br> <i>Computing Surveys</i>, <b>19</b>, 329-358. <br>Looks at the characteristics of reading from a screen and how, by altering them, <br>readability can increase or decrease. It has a good section that compares reading <br>from a paper copy versus reading from a screen. <br> <br>Morse, P. M. (1970). Search theory and browsing. <i>The Library Quarterly</i>, <b>40</b>, 391-<br> 408. <br>Discusses and evaluates an optimal procedure for browsing using previously <br>developed search theories. It then looks at how this can be implemented in a <br>library situation so as to produce high interest browsing. The article includes a <br>lot of numerical analysis and use of theoretical equations to evaluate browsing. <br> <br>Muter, P., and Maurutto, P. (1991). Reading and skimming from computer screens <br> and books: the paperless office revisited? <i>Behaviour &amp; Information Technology</i>, <br><b>10</b>, 257-266. <br>An article that is not directly related to reading behaviour although it does give an <br>insight into the implications on online reading and skimming of documents. <br> <br>Norman, D. A. (1993). <i>Things that make us smart: defending human attributes in the </i><br> <i>age of the machine</i>. Addison-Wesley, Massachusetts. <br>This book reassures readers that the computer is not as intelligent as them. A fun <br>and easy book to read but only mildly relevant to the topic. It does include a <br>small bit on the positives and negatives of reading medium. <br> <br>Oborne, D. J., and Holton, D. (1988). Reading from screen versus paper: there is no <br> difference. <i>International Journal of Man-Machine Studies</i>, <b>28</b>, 1-9. <br>Uses reading and comprehension to compare reading from a computer screen to <br>reading off a paper copy. It indicates that these two factors are not the only <br>factors that need to be considered. Includes a good set of references on this topic. <br> <br> <hr> <A name=25></a>PITCR (Panel on Information Technology and the Conduct of Research). (1989). <br> <i>Information technology and the conduct of research: the users view</i>. National <br>Academy Press, Washington D.C. <br>Discusses the conduct of research under electronic media. It gives an insight into <br>the opportunities and problems that are associated with each area of research <br>when it is conducted electronically. An enlightening book which includes small <br>anecdotes throughout.<i> </i><br> <br>Pinelli, T. E. (1991). The information-seeking habits and practices of engineers. <br> <i>Science and Technology Libraries</i>, <b>12</b>, 5-16. <br>Distinguishes scientists and engineers by their information needs and then <br>proceeds to describe the information needs of engineers in more depth. It is a <br>concise literature review that includes more information about the practices of <br>engineers than their behaviour. <br> <br>Reneker, M. H. (1993). A qualitative study of information seeking among members of <br> an academic community: methodology issues and problems. <i>The Library <br>Quarterly</i>, <b>63</b>, 487-507. <br>Outlines the advantages of doing a qualitative study into information seeking <br>behaviours. It compares the benefits of qualitative and quantitative studies. <br>Unfortunately, no results were concluded that relate to the topic under <br>consideration. <br> <br>Sandstrom, P. E. (1994). An optimal foraging approach to information seeking and <br> use. <i>The Library Quarterly</i>, <b>64</b>, 414-449. <br>An argument putting forward the theory of the optimal forging approach to model <br>information seeking behaviour. Difficult reading with a lot of deductions. <br> <br>Savolainen, R. (1995). Everyday life information seeking: approaching information <br> seeking in the context of “way of life”. <i>Library &amp; Information Science Research</i>, <br><b>17</b>, 259-294. <br>Indicates that it discusses the information seeking practices of individuals, <br>however, it includes a lot of definitions and theories that needs to be waded <br>through. It delves into a whole lot of theory on “the way of life” (order of things) <br>and “mastery of life” (keeping things in order). <br> <br>Schamber, L. (1994). Relevance and information behaviour. <i>Annual Review of </i><br> <i>Information Science and Technology</i>, <b>29</b>, 3-48. <br>A long article on how individuals determine relevance of retrieved material. It <br>defines relevance and the problems with its use in research. Has a good <br>background section on past research into human information behaviour. <br> <br>Seggern, M. V. (1995). Scientists, information seeking and reference services. In <br> <i>Library users and reference services</i>, (ed. J. B. Whitlatch), pp. 95-104. <br>Discusses the information needs and sources for scientists and the implications it <br>has towards library reference services. It includes a summary of works on <br>scientists’ information seeking habits and conclusions and recommendations <br>drawn from them in relation to the library. <br> <br> <hr> <A name=26></a>Showstack, R. (1982). Printing: the next stage: discourse punctuation. In <i>The </i><br> <i>technology of text</i>, (ed. D. H. Jonassen), pp. 369-376. Educational Technology <br>Publications, New Jersey. <br>It compares the two-dimensional structure of text compared to ideas which are <br>multi-dimensional. The article calls to publishers to use discourse punctuation to <br>improve reader understandability, speed, and efficiency. Also it can be used to <br>convey the author’s message more clearly. <br> <br>Sievert, D., and Sievert, M. E. (1989). Philosophical research: report from the field. <br> <i>Proceedings of the Humanists at Work symposium</i> (April, Chicago), pp. 79-94. <br>University of Illinois at Chicago. <br>A commentary about the work “currently” undertaken to determine now <br>philosophers go about researching - the seeking, retrieval and use of relevant <br>information. Easy, light reading, filled with small anecdotes. <br> <br>Snavley, L, and Clark, K. (1996). What users really think: how they see and find <br> serials in the arts and sciences. <i>Library Resources &amp; Technical Services</i>, <b>40</b>, 49-<br>58. <br>The experience of the two authors is presented on how people find and locate <br>serials. It discusses some solutions to problems identified through working as <br>reference librarians. The article calls to librarians to be aware of these problems. <br> <br>Sommer, R. (1966). The ecology of privacy. <i>Library Quarterly</i>, <b>36</b>, 234-248. <br> Discusses the connection between privacy of readers in the library and the <br>physical environment. It covers now people choose their seating position in a <br>library with regard to the current seating arrangement. Interesting reading about <br>reader behaviour in the library, not the behaviour when reading. <br> <br>Sommer, R. (1968). Reading ares in college libraries. <i>Library Quarterly</i>, <b>38</b>, 249-260. <br> Examines the adequacy of reading areas in college libraries as study places. The <br>article looks at the environmental needs and preferences of studiers in libraries <br>and the implications. <br> <br>Sweet, A. P., Riley, R. W., Robinson, S. P., and Conaty, J. C. (1993). <i>State of the art: </i><br> <i>transforming ideas for teaching and learning to read</i>. Found at <br>http://www.ed.gov/pubs/StateArt/covpg.html, on 21 January 1997. <br>A series of ten ideas on how to teach and learn to read. Based in an educational <br>environment, this article offers strategies for reading. Idea eight looks at how <br>expert readers have strategies that they use to construct meaning and is helpful <br>when looking at reading behaviour.<i> </i><br> <br>Tucker, P., and Jones, D. M. (1993). Document annotation: to write, type or speak? <br> <i>International Journal of Man-Machine Studies</i>, <b>39</b>, 885-900. <br>It tries to decide on an acceptable medium for annotating documents online. The <br>article compares written, typed and spoken annotations when proof-reading and <br>refereeing. <br> <br>Wiberley, S., and Jones, W. G. (1989). Patterns of information seeking in the <br> humanities. <i>College &amp; Research Libraries</i>, <b>50</b>, 638-645. <br>An in depth discussion into how humanities scholars go about locating <br> <hr> <A name=27></a>information. It mainly looks at the resources that the scholars use and the <br>implications it has for librarians and libraries. The article does not discuss library <br>behaviour but does have how humanities see libraries. <br> <br>Wilson, T. (1995). <i>Information-seeking behaviour: designing information systems to </i><br> <i>meet out clients’ needs</i>. Found at <br>http://www2.shef.ac.uk/info_studies/public_html/public_html/lecturer/acuril.html<br>, on 6 December 1996. <br>Defines information needs with respect to information seeking behaviour and <br>how this can be implemented in services. A good article that is easy to read. <br> <br>Witten, I. H., Cunningham, S. J., Vallabh, M., and Bell, T. C. (1995). A New Zealand <br> digital library for computer science research. <i>Proc Digital Libraries ’95</i> (April, <br>Texas), pp. 25-30. <br>Gives an overview to the New Zealand Digital Library. Relevant to look at when <br>discussing research behaviour with respect to implications for digital libraries. <br> <br><b>References to material not sighted</b> <br> <br>Barzun, J., and Graff, H. F. (1977). <i>The modern researcher</i>, (3rd edn). Harcourt Brace <br> Jovanocich, New York. <br> <br>Bates, M. (1989). The design of browsing and berrypicking techniques for the online <br> search interface. <i>Online Review</i>, <b>13</b>, 220-228. <br> <br>Bell, W. J. (1991). <i>Searching behaviour: the behavioural ecology of finding </i><br> <i>resources</i>. Chapman and Hall, London. <br> <br>Cooper, W. (1971). A definition of relevance for information retrieval. <i>Information </i><br> <i>storage and retrieval</i>, <b>7</b>, 19-37. <br> <br>De Mul, S., and Oostendorp, H. (1990). Het bestuderen van teksten en het maken van <br> aantekeningen op do computer (studying texts and making notes on the <br>computer). <i>Onderwijsresearchaagen</i>. Technologie en Methodologie, ITS, <br>Nijmegen. <br> <br>Dillon, A. (1994). <i>Designing usable electronic text; ergonomic aspects of human </i><br> <i>information usage</i>. Taylor and Francis, Bristol, PA. <br> <br>Gorman, G. E. (1989). Patterns of information seeking and library use by theologians <br> in seven Adelaide theological colleges. <i>Australian Academic &amp; Research <br>Libraries</i>, <b>50</b>, 638-645. <br> <br>Gould, C. C., Pearce, K. (1991). <i>Information needs in the sciences: an assessment</i>. <br> Research Libraries Group, Mountain View California. <br> <br>Hurd, J. M., Weller A. C., and Karen, L. (1992). Information seeking behaviour of <br> faculty: use of indexes and abstracts by scientists and engineers. <i>American <br>Society of Information Science 55th Annual Meeting</i>, (Pittsburgh). <br> <br> <hr> <A name=28></a>Hyman, R. J. (1972). <i>Access to library collections: an inquiry into the validity of the </i><br> <i>direct shelf approach, with special reference to browsing</i>. Scarecrow Press, New <br>Jersey. <br> <br>Lancester, F. W., and Warner, A. (1985). Electronic publication and its impact on the <br> presentation of information. In <i>The technology of text: principles for structuring, <br>designing, and displaying text</i>, Vol. 2, (ed. D. H. Jonasssen), pp. 292-309. <br>Educational Technology Publications, Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey. <br> <br>Markey, K. (1984). <i>Subject searching in library catalogues before and after the </i><br> <i>introduction of online catalogues</i>. OCLC, Online Computer Library Centre, <br>Dublin, Ohio. <br> <br>McGregor, J. H. (1994). Information seeking and use: students’ thinking and their <br> mental models. <i>Journal of Youth Services in Libraries</i>, <b>8</b>, 69-76. <br> <br>Mosenthal, P. B. (1996). Understanding the strategies of document literacy and their <br> conditions of use. <i>Journal of Educational psychology</i>, <b>88</b>, 314-332. <br> <br>Paris, S. G., Wasik, B. A., and Turner, J. C. (1991). The development of reading <br> strategies. In <i>Handbook of research in the english language arts</i>, (ed. J. Flood, J. <br>M. Jensen, D. Lapp, and J Squire), pp. 609-635, Macmillan, New York. <br> <br>Odini, C. (1993). Trends in information needs and use research. <i>Library Review</i>, <b>42</b>, <br> 29-37. <br> <br>Radecki, T. (1988). Trends in research on information retrieval - the potential for <br> improvements in conventional boolean retrieval systems. <i>Information Processing <br>&amp; Management</i>, <b>24</b>, 219-227. <br> <br>Saracevic, T. (1975). Relevance: a review of a framework for thinking on the notion <br> of information science. <i>Journal of the American Society for Information Science</i>, <br><b>26</b>, 178-194. <br> <br>Saracevic, T., Kantor, P., Chamis, A. Y., and Trivison, D. (1988). A study of <br> information seeking and retrieving. <i>Journal of the American Society for <br>Information Science</i>, <b>39</b>, 161-216. <br> <br>Steinke, C. A. (ed.) (1991). <i>Information seeking and communicating behaviour of </i><br> <i>scientists and engineers</i>. Haworth Press, United States. <br> <br>Van Leunen, M. (1986). <i>A Handbook for scholars</i>. Alfred A Knopf, New York. <br> <br>Wettler, M. (1996). Information processing in information retrieval from the <br> viewpoint of associanist and cognitive psychology. <i>Review of Information <br>Science</i>, <b>1</b>. <br> <br> <hr>